The importance of response times measures in verbal fluency tasks


Published: Feb 9, 2024
Keywords:
Verbal fluency Response times Lexical access Education
Emilia Orologa
https://orcid.org/0009-0004-5395-7524
Georgios Chatzopoulos
Dimitrios Nikolaidis
https://orcid.org/0009-0004-5458-1231
Mary Kosmidis
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8790-1220
Hariklia Proios
Abstract

 


Verbal fluency is a commonly used task in clinical and experimental neuropsychology. It assesses a person’s ability to generate relevant words, according to a given category within a limited amount of time. Phonemic (PF) and semantic (SF) fluency tasks reflect different aspects of language and executive function abilities. In the present study, we investigated first response latency as an additional measure of phonemic, semantic and excluded letter fluency of 44 healthy adults (aged 18 to 39 years old) divided in 2 groups, according to their education level. We investigated potential correlations between response times and education level. Preliminary results from a small sample show that when comparing phonemic, semantic and excluded letter tasks the shortest response time was observed in semantic fluency tasks whereas the excluded letter tasks have the longest (Z = -5.35, p < 0.0005). Education appeared to have a significant negative effect on the mean response times of the participants (for PF U = 137, p = 0.014; for ELF U = 141.5, p = 0.018; for SF t = 2.05, p = 0.046). These results constitute a potential underexamined way to investigate lexical organization and access in verbal fluency. Also, we noted that education contributes not only to overall performance but to response times as well.

Article Details
  • Section
  • RESEARCH PAPERS
Downloads
Download data is not yet available.
References
Acevedo, A., Loewenstein, D. A., Barker, W. W., Harwood, D. G., Luis, C., Bravo, M., Hurwitz, D., Aguero, H., Greenfield, L., & Duara, R. (2000). Category fluency test: normative data for English-and Spanish-speaking elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 760-769. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032
Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solis, F., & Mendoza, V. U. (2000). Learning to read is much more than learning to read: A neuropsychologically based reading program. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(7), 789-801. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677068 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677068
Ayers, M. R., Bushnell, J., Gao, S., Unverzagt, F., Gaizo, J. D., Wadley, V. G., Kennedy, R., & Clark, D. G. (2022). Verbal fluency response times predict incident cognitive impairment. Alzheimer's & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 14(1), e12277. https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12277 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12277
Balogh, R., Imre, N., Gosztolya, G., Pákáski, M., & Kálmán, J. (2022). The Role of Silence in Verbal Fluency Tasks–A New Approach for the Detection of Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721001454 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617721001454
Birn, R. M., Kenworthy, L., Case, L., Caravella, R., Jones, T. B., Bandettini, P. A., & Martin, A. (2010). Neural systems supporting lexical search guided by letter and semantic category cues: a self-paced overt response fMRI study of verbal fluency. Neuroimage, 49(1),1099-1107. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.036 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.036
Bortfeld, H., Leon, S. D., Bloom, J. E., Schober, M. F., & Brennan, S. E. (2001). Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role, and gender. Language and speech, 44(2), 123-147. http://doi.org/10.1177/00238309010440020101 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309010440020101
Crawford, J. R., Wright, R., & Bate, A. (1995). Verbal, figural and ideational fluency in CHI. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 321.
Crowe, S. F. (1998). Decrease in performance on the verbal fluency test as a function of time: Evaluation in a young healthy sample. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 20(3), 391-401. http://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.20.3.391.810 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.20.3.391.810
Da Silva, C. G., Petersson, K. M., Faísca, L., Ingvar, M., & Reis, A. (2004). The effects of literacy and education on the quantitative and qualitative aspects of semantic verbal fluency. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 26(2), 266-277. http://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.2.266.28089 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.2.266.28089
DeAnda, S., Hendrickson, K., Zesiger, P., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Friend, M. (2018). Lexical access in the second year: a study of monolingual and bilingual vocabulary development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 314-327. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000220 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000220
Demetriou, E., & Holtzer, R. (2017). Mild cognitive impairments moderate the effect of time on verbal fluency performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(1), 44-55. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000825 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000825
Gabrić, P., & Vandek, M. (2022). Performance on verbal fluency tasks depends on the given category/letter: Preliminary data from a multivariable analysis. medRxiv, 2021-12. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.12.30.21268567 DOI: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jykpu
Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 18(4), 621. http://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.621 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.621
Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H. (2004). Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 42(9), 1212-1222. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
Holtzer, R., Goldin, Y., Zimmerman, M., Katz, M., Buschke, H., & Lipton, R. B. (2008). Robust norms for selected neuropsychological tests in older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(5), 531-541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2008.05.004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2008.05.004
Hughes, D. L., & Bryan, J. (2002). Adult age differences in strategy use during verbal fluency performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(5), 642-654. http://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.5.642.1002 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.5.642.1002
Kosmidis, M. H., Vlahou, C. H., Panagiotaki, P., & Kiosseoglou, G. (2004). The verbal fluency task in the Greek population: Normative data, and clustering and switching strategies. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 164-172. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102014 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102014
Kraan, C., Stolwyk, R. J., & Testa, R. (2013). The abilities associated with verbal fluency performance in a young, healthy population are multifactorial and differ across fluency variants. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 20(3), 159-168. http://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2012.670157 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2012.670157
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and brain sciences, 22(1), 1-38. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776
Li, Y., Li, P., Yang, Q. X., Eslinger, P. J., Sica, C. T., & Karunanayaka, P. (2017). Lexical-semantic search under different covert verbal fluency tasks: an fMRI study. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 11, 131. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00131 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00131
Libon, D. J., McMillan, C., Gunawardena, D., Powers, C., Massimo, L., Khan, A., Morgan, B., Farag, C., Richmond, L., Weinstein, J., Moore, P, Coslett, H. B., Chatterjee, A., Aguirre, G., & Grossman, M. (2009). Neurocognitive contributions to verbal fluency deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology, 73(7), 535-542. http://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b2a4f5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b2a4f5
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114(1), 29-41. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014
Mainz, N., Shao, Z., Brysbaert, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2017). Vocabulary knowledge predicts lexical processing: Evidence from a group of participants with diverse educational backgrounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1164. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01164 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01164
Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Lalonde, F., & Mack, C. (1994). Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues: A double dissociation in normal subjects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia, 32(12),1487-1494. http://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90120-1 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90120-1
Nogueira, D. S., Reis, E. A., & Vieira, A. (2016). Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 68(3), 124-133. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
Price, J., Gill, D. L., Etnier, J., & Kornatz, K. (2009). Free-throw shooting during dual-task performance: Implications for attentional demand and performance. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 80(4), 718-726. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2009.10599613 DOI: https://doi.org/10.5641/027013609X13088509982441
Raboutet, C., Sauzéon, H., Corsini, M. M., Rodrigues, J., Langevin, S., & N'kaoua, B. (2010). Performance on a semantic verbal fluency task across time: Dissociation between clustering, switching, and categorical exploitation processes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 268-280. http://doi.org/10.1080/13803390902984464 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390902984464
Rosen, V. M., & Engle, R. W. (1997). The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(3), 211. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.126.3.211 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.126.3.211
Shao, Z., Janse, E., Visser, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 772. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772
Shores, E. A., Carstairs, J. R., & Crawford, J. R. (2006). Excluded Letter Fluency Test (ELF): norms and test–retest reliability data for healthy young adults. Brain Impairment, 7(1), 26-32. http://doi.org/10.1375/brim.7.1.26 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1375/brim.7.1.26
Tallberg, I. M., Ivachova, E., Jones Tinghag, K., & Östberg, P. (2008). Swedish norms for word fluency tests: FAS, animals and verbs. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 49(5), 479-485. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00653.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00653.x
Thiele, K., Quinting, J. M., & Stenneken, P. (2016). New ways to analyze word generation performance in brain injury: A systematic review and meta-analysis of additional performance measures. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38(7), 764-781. http://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1163327 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1163327
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138. http://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.11.1.138
Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2011). Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 447-466. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.505292 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.505292
Whiteside, D. M., Kealey, T., Semla, M., Luu, H., Rice, L., Basso, M. R., & Roper, B. (2016). Verbal fluency: language or executive function measure?. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 23(1), 29-34. http://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1004574 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1004574
Wolters, M. K., Kim, N., Kim, J. H., MacPherson, S. E., & Park, J. C. (2016). Prosodic and Linguistic Analysis of Semantic Fluency Data: A Window into Speech Production and Cognition. In Interspeech (pp. 2085-2089). http://doi.org/10.21437/Interspeech.2016-420 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21437/Interspeech.2016-420
Zhao, Q., Guo, Q., & Hong, Z. (2013). Clustering and switching during a semantic verbal fluency test contribute to differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment. Neuroscience bulletin, 29(1), 75-82. http://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-013-1301-7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-013-1301-7
Most read articles by the same author(s)