The use of commutative principle as a shortcut in young children’s mental additive computations


Published: Sep 4, 2019
Keywords:
commutativity principle mental calculations primary school mathematics
Δέσποινα (Despina) Δεσλή (Desli)
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8532-591X
Χαρίκλεια-Ειρήνη Αγγέλη
Abstract

The aim of the present study was to examine young children’s use of commutativity principle in addition problems and its connection to mental calculations. For this aim, a total of 100 children, coming equally from Year 2 and Year 3 classes, were presented with four tasks that involved: a) additions and subtractions favoring sums that give ten, b) additions and subtractions favoring sums with double numbers, c) control additions and subtractions, and d) equivalences with additions and subtractions. Commutativity principle could be used in all tasks, but the third. Results showed that all the participants had high rates of success (82%). Their performance was not affected by age. The majority of the participants used the commutativity strategy in the tasks that its use was allowed and, particularly, in tasks that involved multi-digit addends. Mental calculations were mainly used by all children compared to written calculations. However, Year 2 children showed greater preference to mental computations compared to Year 3 children. 

Article Details
  • Section
  • Articles
Downloads
Download data is not yet available.
Author Biographies
Δέσποινα (Despina) Δεσλή (Desli), Aristotle University of Thessaloniki
Αναπληρώτρια Καθηγήτρια Π.Τ.Δ.Ε. Α.Π.Θ.
Χαρίκλεια-Ειρήνη Αγγέλη, Αριστοτέλειο Πανεπιστήμιο Θεσσαλονίκης Παιδαγωγικό Τμήμα Δημοτικής Εκπαίδευσης
Απόφοιτος ΠΤΔΕ ΑΠΘ
References
Asghari, A.H., & Khosroshahi, L.G. (2017). Making associativity operational. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 15, 1559-1577.
Baroody, A.J., Lai, M., Li, X., & Baroody, A.E. (2009). Preschoolers’ understanding of subtraction-related principles. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 41-60.
Canobi, K.H. (2005). Children’s profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 220-246.
Canobi, K.H. (2004). Individual differences in children’s addition and subtraction knowledge. Cognitive Development, 19, 81-93.
Ching, B.H., & Nunes, T. (2018). Children’s understanding of the commutativity and complement principles: A latent profile analysis. Learning and Instruction, 47, 65-79.
Cowan, R., & Renton, M. (1996). Do they know what they are doing? Children’s use of economical addition strategies and knowledge of commutativity. Educational Psychology, 16(4), 407-420.
Crooks, N.M., & Alibali, M.W. (2014). Defining and measuring conceptual knowledge in mathematics. Developmental Review, 34(4), 344-377.
Author (2011). The use of inversion principle by young children as evidence for conceptual understanding in additive problems. In N. Stellakis, & M. Efstathiadou (Eds.), OMEP European Conference Proceedings (pp. 82-89). Nicosia, Cyprus.
Dube, A.K., & Robinson, K.M. (2010). The relationship between adults’ conceptual understanding of inversion and associativity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(1), 60-66.
Gaschler, R., Vatterodt, B., French, P.A, Eichler, A., & Haider, H. (2013). Spontaneous usage of different shortcuts based on the commutativity principle. Plos One, 8(9). Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074972
Gilmore, C.K., & Bryant, P. (2006). Individual differences in children’s understanding of inversion and arithmetical skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 309-331.
Gilmore, C.K., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009). Patterns of individual differences in conceptual understanding and arithmetical skill: A meta-analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 25-40.
Hiebert, J., & Lefevre, P. (1986). Procedural and conceptual knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Procedural and conceptual knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 1-27). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Kamii, C. & Dominick, A. (1997). To teach or not to teach algorithms. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 16, 51-61.
Klein, & J.S., Bisanz, J. (2000). Preschoolers doing arithmetic: The concepts are willing but the working memory is weak. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 105-115.
McIntosh, A., Reys, B., & Reys, R. (1992). A proposed framework for examining basic number sense. For the Learning of Mathematics, 12(3), 2-8.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Hallet, D., Bell, D., & Evans, D. (2009). Teaching children about the inverse relation between addition and subtraction. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 61-78.
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics. In R. Cohen Kadosh, & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 1102-1118). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Robinson K.M. (2017). The understanding of additive and multiplicative arithmetic concepts. In D.C. Geary, D.B. Berch, R.J. Ochsendorf, & K.M. Koepke (Eds.), Acquisition of complex arithmetic skills and higher-order mathematics concepts (pp. 21-46). Elsevier, Academic Press.
Robinson, K.M., Dube, A.K., & Beatch, J-A. (2017). Children’s understanding of additive concepts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 16-28.
Siegler, R.S., & Lortie-Forgues, H. (2015). Conceptual knowledge of fraction arithmetic. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 909-918.
Star, J.R., & Seifert, C. (2006). The development of flexibity in equation solving. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31(3), 280-300.
van de Walle, Lovin, L.H., Karp, K.S., & Bay-Williams, J.M. (2017). Μαθηματικά από το νηπιαγωγείο ως το Γυμνάσιο. Αθήνα: Gutenberg.
van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (2009). Mathe-didactical reflections on young children’s understanding and application of subtraction-related principles. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11, 102-112.
Wilkins, J.L.M., Baroody, A.J., & Tiilikainen, S. (2001). Kindergartners’ understanding of additive commutativity within the context of word problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 23-36.