The explanatory coexistence of scientific and supernatural explanations: A meta-analysis


Published: Jun 15, 2019
Keywords:
explanatory coexistence the concept of illness origins of life the afterlife
Dimitris Pnevmatikos
Triantafyllia Georgiadou
Abstract

The explanatory coexistence of scientific and supernatural explanations in the same mind challenges the most influential theories of knowledge acquisition in psychology. It shows that although individuals acquire the scientific theories, the supernatural explanations are also used as causal explanatory frameworks even by experts. The present review and meta-analysis aimed to explore the factors that could influence the coexistence of supernatural and scientific explanatory frameworks in the concepts of the origins of life, illness and death/afterlife. On the basis of 35 identified articles (45 studies) that have been published between 1985 and 2016 and that examined both scientific and supernatural explanations within these concepts, the impact of age, religiousness, scientific expertise, cultural background and contextual factors was explored. Results suggest that although religiousness, cultural background, and contextual information have a large effect on the concepts of death/afterlife, illness and the origins of life respectively, the magnitude of the average effect depends on the concept.

Article Details
  • Section
  • RESEARCH PAPERS
Downloads
Download data is not yet available.
References
*Anglin, S. M. (2015). On the nature of implicit soul beliefs: When the past weighs more than the present. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54, 394-404.doi:10.1111/bjso.12094
Aroua, S., Coquide, M., & Abbes, S. (2009). Overcoming the effect of the sociocultural context: Impact of teaching evolution in Tunisia. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 2, 474-478.doi:10.1007/s12052-009-0129-0
*Astuti, R., & Harris, P. L. (2008). Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cognitive Science, 32, 713-740.doi:10.1080/03640210802066907
Babai, R., & Amsterdamer, A. (2008). The persistence of solid and liquid naive conceptions: A reaction time study. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 17, 553-559.doi:10.1007/s10956-008-9122-6
Babai, R., Sekal, R., & Stavy, R. (2010). Persistence of the intuitive conception of living things in adolescence. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19, 20-26.doi:10.1007/s10956-009-9174-2
Baldwin, A. L. (1967). Theories of child development. Oxford, UK:Wiley.
*Bering, J. M. (2002). Intuitive conceptions of dead agents' minds: The natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological boundary. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2, 263-308.doi:10.1163/15685370260441008
*Bering, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The natural emergence of reasoning aboutthe afterlife as a developmental regularity. Developmental Psychology, 40, 217-233.doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.217
*Bering, J. M., Blasi, C. H., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). The development of afterlife beliefs in religiously and secularly schooled children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 587-607.doi: 10.1348/026151005X36498
Berkman, M. B., & Plutzer, E. (2015). Enablers of doubt how future teachers learn tonegotiate the evolution wars in their classrooms. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 658, 253-270.doi: 10.1177/0002716214557783
BouJaoude, S., Asghar, A., Wiles, J. R., Jaber, L., Sarieddine, D., & Alters, B. (2011). Biology professors’ and teachers’ positions regarding biological evolutionand evolution education in a Middle Eastern society. International Journal of Science Education, 33, 979-1000.doi:10.1080/09500693.2010.489124
*Brent, S. B., & Speece, M. W. (1993). “Adult” conceptualization of irreversibility: Implications for the development of the concept of death. Death Studies, 17, 203-224.doi:10.1080/07481189308252618
*Busch, J. T., Watson‐Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2016). The coexistenceof natural and supernatural explanations within and across domains and development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35, 4-20.doi:10.1111/bjdp.12164
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA:BradfordBooks, MIT Press.
Carey, S., Zaitchik, D., & Bascandziev, I. (2015). Theories of development: In dialog with Jean Piaget. Developmental Review, 38, 36-54. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.003
Caravita, S., & Halldén, O. (1994). Re-framing the problem of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 89-111. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(94)90020-5
*Cohen, E., & Barrett, J. (2008). When minds migrate: Conceptualizing spirit possession. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 23-48. doi:10.1163/156770908X289198
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
*Dagher, Z. R., & BouJaoude, S. (1997). Scientific views and religious beliefs of college students: The case of biological evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,34, 429-445. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098 2736(199705)34:5<429::AID-TEA2>3.0.CO;2-S
DeWolf, M., & Vosniadou, S. (2015). The representation of fraction magnitudes and the whole number bias reconsidered. Learning and Instruction, 37, 39-49. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.07.002
Dodick, J., Dayan, A., & Orion, N. (2010). Philosophical approaches of religious Jewish science teachers toward the teaching of ‘controversial’ topics in science. International Journal of Science Education, 32, 1521-1548. doi:10.1080/09500690903518060
*Emmons, N. A., & Kelemen, D. (2014). The development of children's prelife reasoning: Evidence from two cultures. Child Development, 85, 1617-1633. doi:10.1111/cdev.12220
*Emmons, N. A., & Kelemen, D. A. (2015). I’ve got a feeling: Urban and rural indigenous children’s beliefs about early life mentality. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 106-125. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.05.001
*Evans, E. M. (2000). The emergence of beliefs about the origins of species in school-age children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 46, 221-254. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23093715
*Evans, E. M. (2001). Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 217-266. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.074
Evans, E. M., Legare, C. H., & Rosengren, K. (2011). Engaging multiple epistemologies: Implications for science education. In M. Ferrari & R. Taylor (Eds.), Epistemology and science education: Understanding the evolution vs. intelligent design controversy (pp. 111–139). New York, NY: Routledge.
Evans, E. M., Spiegel, A. N., Gram, W., Frazier, B. N., Tare, M., Thompson, S., & Diamond, J. (2010). A conceptual guide to natural history museum visitors' understanding of evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47, 326-353. doi:10.1002/tea.20337
Friedman, H. (1982). Simplified determinations of statistical power, magnitude of effect and research sample sizes. Educational andPsychological Measurement, 42, 521-526. doi:10.1177/001316448204200214
*Furnham, A. (1994). Explaining health and illness: lay beliefs on the nature of health.Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 455-466. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90083-3
*Furnham, A., Akande, D., & Baguma, P. (1999). Beliefs about health and illness inthree countries: Britain, South Africa, and Uganda. Psychology, Health &Medicine, 4, 189-201. doi:10.1002/tea.3660290205
*Furnham, A., & Baguma, P. (1999). Cross-cultural differences in explanations for health and illness: A British and Ugandan comparison. Mental Health,Religion & Culture, 2, 121-134. doi:10.1080/13674679908406341
*Furnham, A., & Igboaka, A. (2007). Young people's recognition and understanding of schizophrenia: A cross-cultural study of young people from Britain and Nigeria. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 53, 430-446. doi:10.1177/0020764007078348
Gelman, S. A., & Legare, C. H. (2011). Concepts and folk theories. Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 379-398. doi:10.1146/annurev-anthro-081309-145822
Goldberg, R. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2009). Developmental “roots” in mature biological knowledge. Psychological Science, 20, 480-487. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02320.x
Gutiérrez, I. T., Rosengren, K. S., & Miller, P. J. (2014). Mexican-American immigrants in the Centerville region: teachers, children, and parents. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 97-112. doi:10.1111/mono.12081
Haddad, M., Waqas, A., Qayyum, W., Shams, M., & Malik, S. (2016). The attitudesand beliefs of Pakistani medical practitioners aboutdepression: Across-sectional study in Lahore using the Revised Depression Attitude Questionnaire (R-DAQ). BMC Psychiatry, 16, 349. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-1069-1
*Harris, P. L., & Giménez, M. (2005). Children's acceptance of conflicting testimony: The case of death. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 143-164. doi:10.1163/1568537054068606
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York, NY:Academic Press.
*Jobanputra, R., & Furnham, A. F. (2005). British Gujarati Indian immigrants' and British Caucasians' beliefs about health and illness. International Journalof Social Psychiatry, 51, 350-364. doi:10.1177/0020764005060851
Kallio, E. (2011). Integrative thinking is the key: An evaluation of current researchinto the development of adult thinking. Theory & Psychology, 21, 785-801. doi:10.1177/0959354310388344
Keikelame, M. J., & Swartz, L. (2015). ‘A thing full of stories’: Traditional healers’ explanations of epilepsy and perspectives on collaboration with biomedical health care in Cape Town. Transcultural Psychiatry, 52, 659-680. doi:10.1177/1363461515571626
Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2013). Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1074-1083.doi:10.1037/a0030399
*Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1994). Cultural diversity in causal attributions for illness: The role of the supernatural. Journal of BehavioralMedicine, 17, 181-193.doi:10.1007/BF01858104
Lane, J. D., & Harris, P. L. (2014). Confronting, representing, and believing counterintuitive conceptsnavigating the natural and the supernatural. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 144-160. doi:10.1177/1745691613518078
*Lane, J. D., Zhu, L., Evans, E. M., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Developing conceptsof the mind, body, and afterlife: Exploring the roles of narrative context and culture. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 16, 50-82. doi:10.1163/15685373-12342168
*Lawson, A. E., & Worsnop, W. A. (1992). Learning about evolution and rejecting a belief in special creation: Effects of reflective reasoning skill, prior knowledge, prior belief and religious commitment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29, 143-166. doi:10.1002/tea.3660290205
Legare, C. H., Evans, E. M., Rosengren, K. S., & Harris, P. L. (2012). The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Development, 83, 779-793. doi:10.1111/j.14678624.2012.01743.x
*Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Bewitchment, biology, or both: The co-existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development. Cognitive Science, 32, 607-642. doi:10.1080/03640210802066766
Legare, C. H., & Shtulman, A. (2018). Explanatory pluralism across cultures and development. In J. Proust & M. Fortier (Eds.), Interdisciplinary approaches to metacognitive diversity (pp. 415-432). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P.,& Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151, 65-94. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00136
*Losh, S. C., & Nzekwe, B. (2011). Creatures in the classroom: preservice teacher beliefs about fantastic beasts, magic, extraterrestrials, evolution,and creationism. Science & Education, 20, 473-489. doi:10.1007/s11191-010-9268-5
Mansour, N. (2011). Science teachers' views of science and religion vs. the Islamic perspective: Conflicting or compatible? Science Education, 95, 281-309. doi:10.1002/sce.20418
Makris, N., Tachmatzidis, D., Demetriou, A., & Spanoudis, G. (2017). Mapping the evolving core of intelligence: Changing relations between executive control, reasoning, language, and awareness. Intelligence, 62, 12-30. doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.01.006
Mason, L., Zaccoletti, S., Carretti, B., Scrimin, S., & Diakidoy, I. A. N. (2018). The role of inhibition in conceptual learning from refutation and standard expository texts. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 1-19. doi:10.1007/s10763-017-9874-7
*Mathews, M. (2011). Assessment and comparison of culturally based explanations for mental disorder among Singaporean Chinese youth. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 57, 3-17. doi:10.1177/0020764008096853
*Nguyen, S. P., & Rosengren, K. S. (2004). Causal reasoning about illness: A comparison between European-and Vietnamese-American children. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 51-78. doi:10.1163/156853704323074750
Ohlsson, S. (2013). Beyond evidence-based belief formation: How normative ideas have constrained conceptual change research. Frontline Learning Research, 1, 70-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.14786/flr.v1i2.58
*Opfer, J. E., Nehm, R. H., & Ha, M. (2012). Cognitive foundations for science assessment design: Knowing what students know about evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 49, 744-777. doi:10.1002/tea.21028
*Panagiotaki, G., Nobes, G., Ashraf, A., & Aubby, H. (2014). British and Pakistani children's understanding of death: Cultural and developmental influences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 31-44. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12064
Piaget, J. (2010). La causalité chez l’énfant (Children's understanding of causality). British Journal of Psychology, 100, 207-224. (Original article published 1928) doi:10.1348/000712608X336059.
Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 175-181. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175
Pnevmatikos, D.,& Makris, N. (2010). Cross-cultural evidence for constraints in conceptual development, or, are the places where Allah and God live the same?In J. Håkansson(Ed.), Developmental Psychology, (pp.117-133). New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers,
Pnevmatikos, D.,& Makris, N. (2011). Concept of God: Conceptual change and mental models during childhood and adolescence. Psychology, 18(1), 1-19.
Pnevmatikos, D. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for religious thinking in a Greek-Orthodox sample. Psychology, 7(1), 20-34.
Pnevmatikos, D. (2002). Conceptual change, mental models and their internal consistency within the religious domain of knowledge. In S. Lehti &K. Merenluoto (Eds.), A process approach to conceptual change (pp. 148-158). Turku, University of Turku.
*Pnevmatikos, D. (2014). Prayer as a cause of recovery from illness. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 35, 218-221. doi:10.1080/13617672.2014.953304
Poling, D. A., & Evans, E. M. (2004 a). Religious belief, scientific expertise, and folk ecology. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 485-524. doi:10.1163/1568537042484931
*Poling, D. A., & Evans, E. M. (2004 b). Are dinosaurs the rule or the exception? Developing concepts of death and extinction.Cognitive Development, 19, 363-383. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.04.001
Potvin, P., Masson, S., Lafortune, S., & Cyr, G. (2015). Persistence of the intuitiveconception that heavier objects sink more: A reaction time study with different levels of interference. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 13, 21-43. doi:10.1007/s10763-014-9520-6
*Preston, J. L., Ritter, R. S., & Hepler, J. (2013). Neuroscience and the Soul:competing explanations for the human experience. Cognition, 127, 31-37. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.003
*Raman, L., & Gelman, S. A. (2004). A cross-cultural developmental analysis of children's and adults' understanding of illness in South Asia (India) and the United States. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4, 293-317. doi:10.1163/1568537041725088
*Richert, R. A., & Harris, P. L. (2006). The ghost in my body: Children's developing concept of the soul. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 6, 409-427. doi:10.1163/156853706778554913
Rosengren, K. S., Gutiérrez, I. T., & Schein, S. S. (2014a). Cognitive dimensions of death in context. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 62-82. doi:10.1111/mono.12079
Rosengren, K. S., Gutiérrez, I. T., & Schein, S. S. (2014b). Cognitive modelsof death. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 79, 83-96. doi:10.1111/mono.12080
Saroglou, V. (2010). Religiousness as a cultural adaptation of basic traits: A five-factor model perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 108-125. doi: 10.1177/1088868309352322
*Short, S. D., & Hawley, P. H. (2015). The effects of evolution education: Examining attitudes toward and knowledge of evolution in college courses. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 67-88. doi:10.1177/147470491501300105
Shtulman, A. (2013). Epistemic similarities between students’ scientific and supernatural beliefs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 199-212. doi:10.1037/a0030282
Shtulman, A., & Harrington, K. (2016). Tensions between science and intuition across the lifespan. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 118-137. doi:10.1111/tops.12174
Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209-215.doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005
Shweder, R., Much, N., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “big three” of Morality (Autonomy, Community, Divinity)and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In A.M. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119-169). New York, NY: Routledge.
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach.Cambridge, MA: Cambridge.
Stathopoulou, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2007). Exploring the relationship between physics-related epistemological beliefs and physics understanding.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 255-281. doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.12.002
Tabi, M. M., Powell, M., & Hodnicki, D. (2006). Use of traditional healers and modern medicine in Ghana. International Nursing Review, 53, 52-58. doi:10.1111/j.1466-7657.2006.00444.x
*Vess, M., Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Routledge, C., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2009). Exploring the existential function of religion: The effect of religious fundamentalism and mortality salience on faith-based medical refusals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 334-350. doi:10.1037/a0015545
Vosniadou, S., Pnevmantikos, D., Makris, N., Ikospentaki, K., Lepenioti, D., Chountala, A., & Kyrianakis, G. (2015). Executive functions and conceptual change in science and mathematics. In D. C. Noele, R. Dale, A.S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Mattlock, D.C. Jennings, & P.P. Maglio (Eds.),Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2529-2534). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Vosniadou, S., Pnevmatikos, D., Makris, N., Lepenioti, D., Eikospentaki, K., Chountala, A., &Kyrianakis, G. (2018). The recruitment of shifting and inhibition in on-linescience and mathematics tasks. Cognitive Science. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12624
Vosniadou, S., Pnevmatikos, D., &Makris, N. (2018).The role of executive function in the construction and employment of scientific and athematical concepts that require conceptual change learning. Neuroeducation Journal: Special Issue on Executive Functions and Academic Learning, 6.
Watson‐Jones, R. E., Busch, J. T., Harris, P. L., & Legare, C. H. (2016). Does the body survive death? Cultural variation in beliefs about life everlasting. Cognitive Science, 41, 455-476. doi:10.1111/cogs.12430
Watson‐Jones, R. E., Busch, J. T., & Legare, C. H. (2015). Interdisciplinary and cross‐cultural perspectives on explanatory coexistence. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7, 611-623. doi:10.1111/tops.12162
Winslow, M. W., Staver, J. R., & Scharmann, L. C. (2011). Evolution and personal religious belief: Christian university biology related majors' search for reconciliation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48, 1026-1049. doi:10.1002/tea.20417
Wolf, F. (1986). Meta-analysis: Quantitative methods for research synthesis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Woolley, J. D., & Ghossainy, M. (2013). Revisiting the fantasy-reality distinction: Children as naïve skeptics.Child Development, 84, 1496-1510. doi:10.1111/cdev.12081
Most read articles by the same author(s)